- -

Effect of an upright (vs. stooped) posture on interpretation bias, imagery, and emotions

RiuNet: Repositorio Institucional de la Universidad Politécnica de Valencia

Compartir/Enviar a

Citas

Estadísticas

  • Estadisticas de Uso

Effect of an upright (vs. stooped) posture on interpretation bias, imagery, and emotions

Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem

Ficheros en el ítem

dc.contributor.author Miragall, Marta es_ES
dc.contributor.author Borrego, Adrián es_ES
dc.contributor.author CEBOLLA, AUSIAS es_ES
dc.contributor.author ETCHEMENDY, ERNESTINA es_ES
dc.contributor.author Navarro-Siurana, Jésica es_ES
dc.contributor.author Llorens Rodríguez, Roberto es_ES
dc.contributor.author Blackwell, Simon E. es_ES
dc.contributor.author Baños Rivera, Rosa María es_ES
dc.date.accessioned 2021-04-27T03:33:11Z
dc.date.available 2021-04-27T03:33:11Z
dc.date.issued 2020-09 es_ES
dc.identifier.issn 0005-7916 es_ES
dc.identifier.uri http://hdl.handle.net/10251/165609
dc.description.abstract [EN] Background and objectives: Adopting an upright (vs. stooped) posture has been related to positive effects on emotional and cognitive processes. However, there is no evidence concerning the effect of posture on two key processes associated with the maintenance of depression: interpretation bias and vividness of mental imagery. The objectives were to investigate the effect of adopting an upright (vs. stooped) posture on interpretation bias and vividness of positive and negative mental imagery, and to explore the interplay between these processes and depression-related emotions. Methods: The sample consisted of 54 participants (M-age = 22.00, 64.8% women), who were randomly assigned to the upright or stooped condition. Participants answered self-report measures while they were adopting a specific posture. Posture was monitored through inertial technology. Results: Main results were that upright (vs. stooped) posture led to more positive interpretations of ambiguous information and increased positive emotions related to depression (happiness, optimism and vigor); time in an upright position was associated with change in interpretation bias and vividness of positive mental imagery; and level of depressive symptomatology moderated the effect of posture on the change in interpretation bias. Limitations: Limitations are related to the use of non-clinical sample, the use of short-term measurements, and the lack of an experimental condition adopting the usual posture. Conclusions: Posture interacts with mechanisms involved in the maintenance of depression, as well as with depression-related emotions. This study has clinical implications that should be continued explored in order to clarify the role of manipulating the posture in individuals with depressive symptomatology. es_ES
dc.description.sponsorship This work was supported by the Ministerio de Economia, Industria y Competitividad of Spain under grant "AN-BODYMENT" (PSI2017-85063-R), and BES-2014-068218; and by Universitat Politecnica de Valencia (Grant PAID-10-16). The funding source had no involvement in the study design, in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data, in the writing of the report, or in the decision to submit the article for publication. es_ES
dc.language Inglés es_ES
dc.publisher Elsevier es_ES
dc.relation.ispartof Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry es_ES
dc.rights Reconocimiento - No comercial - Sin obra derivada (by-nc-nd) es_ES
dc.subject Interpretation bias es_ES
dc.subject Mental imagery es_ES
dc.subject Upright posture es_ES
dc.subject Stooped posture es_ES
dc.subject Depression es_ES
dc.subject.classification INGENIERIA TELEMATICA es_ES
dc.subject.classification TEORIA DE LA SEÑAL Y COMUNICACIONES es_ES
dc.title Effect of an upright (vs. stooped) posture on interpretation bias, imagery, and emotions es_ES
dc.type Artículo es_ES
dc.identifier.doi 10.1016/j.jbtep.2020.101560 es_ES
dc.relation.projectID info:eu-repo/grantAgreement/MINECO//BES-2014-068218/ES/BES-2014-068218/ es_ES
dc.relation.projectID info:eu-repo/grantAgreement/AEI/Plan Estatal de Investigación Científica y Técnica y de Innovación 2013-2016/PSI2017-85063-R/ES/ANOREXIA NERVIOSA Y CUERPO: EVALUACION Y MODIFICACION MEDIANTE REALIDAD VIRTUAL DE LAS REPRESENTACIONES MENTALES DEL CUERPO/ es_ES
dc.relation.projectID info:eu-repo/grantAgreement/UPV//PAID-10-16/ es_ES
dc.rights.accessRights Abierto es_ES
dc.contributor.affiliation Universitat Politècnica de València. Instituto Interuniversitario de Investigación en Bioingeniería y Tecnología Orientada al Ser Humano - Institut Interuniversitari d'Investigació en Bioenginyeria i Tecnologia Orientada a l'Ésser Humà es_ES
dc.description.bibliographicCitation Miragall, M.; Borrego, A.; Cebolla, A.; Etchemendy, E.; Navarro-Siurana, J.; Llorens Rodríguez, R.; Blackwell, SE.... (2020). Effect of an upright (vs. stooped) posture on interpretation bias, imagery, and emotions. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 68:1-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2020.101560 es_ES
dc.description.accrualMethod S es_ES
dc.relation.publisherversion https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2020.101560 es_ES
dc.description.upvformatpinicio 1 es_ES
dc.description.upvformatpfin 9 es_ES
dc.type.version info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion es_ES
dc.description.volume 68 es_ES
dc.identifier.pmid 32078947 es_ES
dc.relation.pasarela S\404104 es_ES
dc.contributor.funder Universitat Politècnica de València es_ES
dc.contributor.funder Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad es_ES
dc.contributor.funder Agencia Estatal de Investigación es_ES
dc.description.references Barnard, P. J., & Teasdale, J. D. (1991). Interacting cognitive subsystems: A systemic approach to cognitive-affective interaction and change. Cognition & Emotion, 5(1), 1-39. doi:10.1080/02699939108411021 es_ES
dc.description.references Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded Cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 617-645. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639 es_ES
dc.description.references Blackwell, S. E., Browning, M., Mathews, A., Pictet, A., Welch, J., Davies, J., … Holmes, E. A. (2015). Positive Imagery-Based Cognitive Bias Modification as a Web-Based Treatment Tool for Depressed Adults. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(1), 91-111. doi:10.1177/2167702614560746 es_ES
dc.description.references Blackwell, S. E., & Holmes, E. A. (2010). Modifying interpretation and imagination in clinical depression: A single case series using cognitive bias modification. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(3), 338-350. doi:10.1002/acp.1680 es_ES
dc.description.references Blackwell, S. E., Westermann, K., Woud, M. L., Cwik, J. C., Neher, T., Graz, C., … Margraf, J. (2018). Computerized positive mental imagery training versus cognitive control training versus treatment as usual in inpatient mental health settings: study protocol for a randomized controlled feasibility trial. Pilot and Feasibility Studies, 4(1). doi:10.1186/s40814-018-0325-1 es_ES
dc.description.references Carney, D. R., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Yap, A. J. (2010). Power Posing. Psychological Science, 21(10), 1363-1368. doi:10.1177/0956797610383437 es_ES
dc.description.references Cuddy, A. J. C., Schultz, S. J., & Fosse, N. E. (2018). P-Curving a More Comprehensive Body of Research on Postural Feedback Reveals Clear Evidential Value for Power-Posing Effects: Reply to Simmons and Simonsohn (2017). Psychological Science, 29(4), 656-666. doi:10.1177/0956797617746749 es_ES
dc.description.references Cuddy, A. J. C., Wilmuth, C. A., Yap, A. J., & Carney, D. R. (2015). Preparatory power posing affects nonverbal presence and job interview performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(4), 1286-1295. doi:10.1037/a0038543 es_ES
dc.description.references Everaert, J., Koster, E. H. W., & Derakshan, N. (2012). The combined cognitive bias hypothesis in depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(5), 413-424. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.04.003 es_ES
dc.description.references Everaert, J., Podina, I. R., & Koster, E. H. W. (2017). A comprehensive meta-analysis of interpretation biases in depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 58, 33-48. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.005 es_ES
dc.description.references Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. doi:10.3758/bf03193146 es_ES
dc.description.references Gotlib, I. H., & Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and Depression: Current Status and Future Directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6(1), 285-312. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131305 es_ES
dc.description.references Holmes, E. A., Blackwell, S. E., Burnett Heyes, S., Renner, F., & Raes, F. (2016). Mental Imagery in Depression: Phenomenology, Potential Mechanisms, and Treatment Implications. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 12(1), 249-280. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-092925 es_ES
dc.description.references Holmes, E. A., Lang, T. J., & Deeprose, C. (2009). Mental Imagery and Emotion in Treatment across Disorders: Using the Example of Depression. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 38(sup1), 21-28. doi:10.1080/16506070902980729 es_ES
dc.description.references Holmes, E. A., Lang, T. J., Moulds, M. L., & Steele, A. M. (2008). Prospective and positive mental imagery deficits in dysphoria. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(8), 976-981. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.04.009 es_ES
dc.description.references Ingram, R. E. (1984). Toward an information-processing analysis of depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8(5), 443-477. doi:10.1007/bf01173284 es_ES
dc.description.references Joormann, J., Yoon, K. L., & Zetsche, U. (2007). Cognitive inhibition in depression. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12(3), 128-139. doi:10.1016/j.appsy.2007.09.002 es_ES
dc.description.references Körner, A., Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2015). Routes to embodiment. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00940 es_ES
dc.description.references LeMoult, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2019). Depression: A cognitive perspective. Clinical Psychology Review, 69, 51-66. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2018.06.008 es_ES
dc.description.references Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive Vulnerability to Emotional Disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1(1), 167-195. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143916 es_ES
dc.description.references Nair, S., Sagar, M., Sollers, J., Consedine, N., & Broadbent, E. (2015). Do slumped and upright postures affect stress responses? A randomized trial. Health Psychology, 34(6), 632-641. doi:10.1037/hea0000146 es_ES
dc.description.references Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying Emotion. Science, 316(5827), 1002-1005. doi:10.1126/science.1136930 es_ES
dc.description.references Platt, B., Waters, A. M., Schulte-Koerne, G., Engelmann, L., & Salemink, E. (2016). A review of cognitive biases in youth depression: attention, interpretation and memory. Cognition and Emotion, 31(3), 462-483. doi:10.1080/02699931.2015.1127215 es_ES
dc.description.references Rohrbacher, H., & Reinecke, A. (2014). Measuring Change in Depression-Related Interpretation Bias: Development and Validation of a Parallel Ambiguous Scenarios Test. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 43(3), 239-250. doi:10.1080/16506073.2014.919605 es_ES
dc.description.references Stöber, J. (2000). Prospective cognitions in anxiety and depression: Replication and methodological extension. Cognition & Emotion, 14(5), 725-729. doi:10.1080/02699930050117693 es_ES
dc.description.references Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z., & Williams, J. M. G. (1995). How does cognitive therapy prevent depressive relapse and why should attentional control (mindfulness) training help? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(1), 25-39. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)e0011-7 es_ES
dc.description.references Tsai, H.-Y., Peper, E., & Lin, I.-M. (2016). EEG patterns under positive/negative body postures and emotion recall tasks. NeuroRegulation, 3(1), 23-27. doi:10.15540/nr.3.1.23 es_ES
dc.description.references Veenstra, L., Schneider, I. K., & Koole, S. L. (2016). Embodied mood regulation: the impact of body posture on mood recovery, negative thoughts, and mood-congruent recall. Cognition and Emotion, 31(7), 1361-1376. doi:10.1080/02699931.2016.1225003 es_ES
dc.description.references Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063-1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 es_ES
dc.description.references Wilkes, C., Kydd, R., Sagar, M., & Broadbent, E. (2017). Upright posture improves affect and fatigue in people with depressive symptoms. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 54, 143-149. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.07.015 es_ES
dc.description.references Wilson, V. E., & Peper, E. (2004). The Effects of Upright and Slumped Postures on the Recall of Positive and Negative Thoughts. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 29(3), 189-195. doi:10.1023/b:apbi.0000039057.32963.34 es_ES


Este ítem aparece en la(s) siguiente(s) colección(ones)

Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem