Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem
dc.contributor.author | Zarzo Castelló, Manuel | es_ES |
dc.date.accessioned | 2014-09-23T07:51:58Z | |
dc.date.available | 2014-09-23T07:51:58Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2013-01 | |
dc.identifier.issn | 1424-8220 | |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/10251/39854 | |
dc.description.abstract | Perfumes are manufactured by mixing odorous materials with different volatilities. The parameter that measures the lasting property of a material when applied on the skin is called substantivity or tenacity. It is well known by perfumers that citrus and green notes are perceived as fresh and they tend to evaporate quickly, while odors most dissimilar to fresh (e.g., oriental, powdery, erogenic and animalic scents) are tenacious. However, studies aimed at quantifying the relationship between fresh odor quality and substantivity have not received much attention. In this work, perceptual olfactory ratings on a fresh scale, estimated in a previous study, were compared with substantivity parameters and antierogenic ratings from the literature. It was found that the correlation between fresh odor character and odorant substantivity is quite strong (r = −0.85). Fresh is sometimes interpreted in perfumery as cool and the opposite of warm . This association suggests that odor freshness might be somehow related to temperature. Assuming that odor perception space was shaped throughout evolution in temperate climates, results reported here are consistent with the hypothesis that fresh evokes scents typically encountered in the cool season, while warm would be evoked by odors found in nature during summer. This hypothesis is rather simplistic but it may provide a new insight to better understand the perceptual space of scents. | es_ES |
dc.language | Inglés | es_ES |
dc.publisher | MDPI | es_ES |
dc.relation.ispartof | Sensors | es_ES |
dc.rights | Reconocimiento (by) | es_ES |
dc.subject | Fragrance | es_ES |
dc.subject | Tenacity | es_ES |
dc.subject | Odor descriptor | es_ES |
dc.subject | Perceptual dimension | es_ES |
dc.subject | Olfactory map | es_ES |
dc.subject.classification | ESTADISTICA E INVESTIGACION OPERATIVA | es_ES |
dc.title | What is a fresh scent in perfumery? Perceptual freshness iscorrelated with substantivity | es_ES |
dc.type | Artículo | es_ES |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.3390/s130100463 | |
dc.rights.accessRights | Abierto | es_ES |
dc.contributor.affiliation | Universitat Politècnica de València. Departamento de Estadística e Investigación Operativa Aplicadas y Calidad - Departament d'Estadística i Investigació Operativa Aplicades i Qualitat | es_ES |
dc.description.bibliographicCitation | Zarzo Castelló, M. (2013). What is a fresh scent in perfumery? Perceptual freshness iscorrelated with substantivity. Sensors. 13(1):463-483. doi:10.3390/s130100463 | es_ES |
dc.description.accrualMethod | S | es_ES |
dc.relation.publisherversion | http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/s130100463 | es_ES |
dc.description.upvformatpinicio | 463 | es_ES |
dc.description.upvformatpfin | 483 | es_ES |
dc.type.version | info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion | es_ES |
dc.description.volume | 13 | es_ES |
dc.description.issue | 1 | es_ES |
dc.relation.senia | 231922 | |
dc.identifier.pmid | 23275083 | en_EN |
dc.identifier.pmcid | PMC3574685 | en_EN |
dc.description.references | Zarzo, M., & Stanton, D. T. (2009). Understanding the underlying dimensions in perfumers’ odor perception space as a basis for developing meaningful odor maps. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71(2), 225-247. doi:10.3758/app.71.2.225 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Rudolfi, T. A., Schedrina, M. M., & Mindlin, L. O. (1988). Determination of the evaporation rate of essential oils and perfumery compositions using gas chromatography. Chromatographia, 25(6), 520-522. doi:10.1007/bf02324825 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Zarzo, M. (2008). Relevant psychological dimensions in the perceptual space of perfumery odors. Food Quality and Preference, 19(3), 315-322. doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2007.10.007 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Chastrette, M., Laumer, J.-Y. de S., & Sauvegrain, P. (1991). Analysis of a system of description of odors by means of four different multivariate statistical methods. Chemical Senses, 16(1), 81-93. doi:10.1093/chemse/16.1.81 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | The Rosace of Odors (Firmenich, 1972)http://membres.multimania.fr/sensonet/langa/rosace-firmenich.html | es_ES |
dc.description.references | http://www.perfumerbook.com/ Scent%20Pyramid.pdf | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Abe, H., Kanaya, S., Komukai, T., Takahashi, Y., & Sasaki, S. (1990). Systemization of semantic descriptions of odors. Analytica Chimica Acta, 239, 73-85. doi:10.1016/s0003-2670(00)83837-4 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Chastrette, M., Elmouaffek, A., & Sauvegrain, P. (1988). A multidimensional statistical study of similarities between 74 notes used in perfumery. Chemical Senses, 13(2), 295-305. doi:10.1093/chemse/13.2.295 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Wise, P. M. (2000). Quantification of Odor Quality. Chemical Senses, 25(4), 429-443. doi:10.1093/chemse/25.4.429 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Teixeira, M. A., Rodríguez, O., & Rodrigues, A. E. (2010). Perfumery Radar: A Predictive Tool for Perfume Family Classification. Industrial & Engineering Chemistry Research, 49(22), 11764-11777. doi:10.1021/ie101161v | es_ES |
dc.description.references | http://www-crnl.univ-lyon1.fr/documentation/olfaction/classification_des_parfums/sommaire.htm | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Zarzo, M. (2011). Hedonic Judgments of Chemical Compounds Are Correlated with Molecular Size. Sensors, 11(4), 3667-3686. doi:10.3390/s110403667 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Yokoyama, K., & Ebisawa, F. (1993). Detection and evaluation of fragrances by human reactions using a chemical sensor based on adsorbate detection. Analytical Chemistry, 65(6), 673-677. doi:10.1021/ac00054a004 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Stevenson, R. J., & Boakes, R. A. (2003). A mnemonic theory of odor perception. Psychological Review, 110(2), 340-364. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.110.2.340 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Ayabe-Kanamura, S., Schicker, I., Laska, M., Hudson, R., Distel, H., Kobayakawa, T., & Saito, S. (1998). Differences in Perception of Everyday Odors: a Japanese-German Cross-cultural Study. Chemical Senses, 23(1), 31-38. doi:10.1093/chemse/23.1.31 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Chrea, C., Valentin, D., Sulmont-Rossé, C., Ly Mai, H., Hoang Nguyen, D., & Abdi, H. (2004). Culture and odor categorization: agreement between cultures depends upon the odors. Food Quality and Preference, 15(7-8), 669-679. doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2003.10.005 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Kirk-Smith, M. D., & Booth, D. A. (1987). Chemoreception in human behaviour: experimental analysis of the social effects of fragrances. Chemical Senses, 12(1), 159-166. doi:10.1093/chemse/12.1.159 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Schaal, B., Marlier, L., & Soussignan, R. (1998). Olfactory function in the human fetus: Evidence from selective neonatal responsiveness to the odor of amniotic fluid. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112(6), 1438-1449. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.112.6.1438 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Mennella, J. A., Jagnow, C. P., & Beauchamp, G. K. (2001). Prenatal and Postnatal Flavor Learning by Human Infants. PEDIATRICS, 107(6), e88-e88. doi:10.1542/peds.107.6.e88 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Wallace, K. J., & Rosen, J. B. (2000). Predator odor as an unconditioned fear stimulus in rats: Elicitation of freezing by trimethylthiazoline, a component of fox feces. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114(5), 912-922. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.114.5.912 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Rossiter, K. J. (1996). Structure−Odor Relationships. Chemical Reviews, 96(8), 3201-3240. doi:10.1021/cr950068a | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Ferdenzi, C., Schirmer, A., Roberts, S. C., Delplanque, S., Porcherot, C., Cayeux, I., … Grandjean, D. (2011). Affective dimensions of odor perception: A comparison between Swiss, British, and Singaporean populations. Emotion, 11(5), 1168-1181. doi:10.1037/a0022853 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Kobayakawa, K., Kobayakawa, R., Matsumoto, H., Oka, Y., Imai, T., Ikawa, M., … Sakano, H. (2007). Innate versus learned odour processing in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nature, 450(7169), 503-508. doi:10.1038/nature06281 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Chrea, C., Grandjean, D., Delplanque, S., Cayeux, I., Le Calve, B., Aymard, L., … Scherer, K. R. (2008). Mapping the Semantic Space for the Subjective Experience of Emotional Responses to Odors. Chemical Senses, 34(1), 49-62. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjn052 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | PROST, C., GUEN, S. L., COURCOUX, P., & DEMAIMAY, M. (2001). SIMILARITIES AMONG 40 PURE ODORANT COMPOUNDS EVALUATED BY CONSUMERS. Journal of Sensory Studies, 16(6), 551-565. doi:10.1111/j.1745-459x.2001.tb00320.x | es_ES |
dc.description.references | SCHIFFMAN, S., ROBINSON, D. E., & ERICKSON, R. P. (1977). MULTIDIMENSIONAL SCALING OF ODORANTS: EXAMINATION OF PSYCHOLOGICAL AND PHYSICOCHEMICAL DIMENSIONS. Chemical Senses, 2(3), 375-390. doi:10.1093/chemse/2.3.375 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | HARPER, R. (1975). SOME CHEMICALS REPRESENTING PARTICULAR ODOUR QUALITIES. Chemical Senses, 1(3), 353-357. doi:10.1093/chemse/1.3.353 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Stoddart, D. M. (1986). The Role of Olfaction in the Evolution of Human Sexual Biology: An Hypothesis. Man, 21(3), 514. doi:10.2307/2803099 | es_ES |
dc.description.references | Ou, L.-C., Ronnier Luo, M., Sun, P.-L., Hu, N.-C., Chen, H.-S., Guan, S.-S., … Richter, K. (2010). A cross-cultural comparison of colour emotion for two-colour combinations. Color Research & Application, 37(1), 23-43. doi:10.1002/col.20648 | es_ES |