- -

What is a fresh scent in perfumery? Perceptual freshness iscorrelated with substantivity

RiuNet: Repositorio Institucional de la Universidad Politécnica de Valencia

Compartir/Enviar a

Citas

Estadísticas

  • Estadisticas de Uso

What is a fresh scent in perfumery? Perceptual freshness iscorrelated with substantivity

Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem

Ficheros en el ítem

dc.contributor.author Zarzo Castelló, Manuel es_ES
dc.date.accessioned 2014-09-23T07:51:58Z
dc.date.available 2014-09-23T07:51:58Z
dc.date.issued 2013-01
dc.identifier.issn 1424-8220
dc.identifier.uri http://hdl.handle.net/10251/39854
dc.description.abstract Perfumes are manufactured by mixing odorous materials with different volatilities. The parameter that measures the lasting property of a material when applied on the skin is called substantivity or tenacity. It is well known by perfumers that citrus and green notes are perceived as fresh and they tend to evaporate quickly, while odors most dissimilar to fresh (e.g., oriental, powdery, erogenic and animalic scents) are tenacious. However, studies aimed at quantifying the relationship between fresh odor quality and substantivity have not received much attention. In this work, perceptual olfactory ratings on a fresh scale, estimated in a previous study, were compared with substantivity parameters and antierogenic ratings from the literature. It was found that the correlation between fresh odor character and odorant substantivity is quite strong (r = −0.85). Fresh is sometimes interpreted in perfumery as cool and the opposite of warm . This association suggests that odor freshness might be somehow related to temperature. Assuming that odor perception space was shaped throughout evolution in temperate climates, results reported here are consistent with the hypothesis that fresh evokes scents typically encountered in the cool season, while warm would be evoked by odors found in nature during summer. This hypothesis is rather simplistic but it may provide a new insight to better understand the perceptual space of scents. es_ES
dc.language Inglés es_ES
dc.publisher MDPI es_ES
dc.relation.ispartof Sensors es_ES
dc.rights Reconocimiento (by) es_ES
dc.subject Fragrance es_ES
dc.subject Tenacity es_ES
dc.subject Odor descriptor es_ES
dc.subject Perceptual dimension es_ES
dc.subject Olfactory map es_ES
dc.subject.classification ESTADISTICA E INVESTIGACION OPERATIVA es_ES
dc.title What is a fresh scent in perfumery? Perceptual freshness iscorrelated with substantivity es_ES
dc.type Artículo es_ES
dc.identifier.doi 10.3390/s130100463
dc.rights.accessRights Abierto es_ES
dc.contributor.affiliation Universitat Politècnica de València. Departamento de Estadística e Investigación Operativa Aplicadas y Calidad - Departament d'Estadística i Investigació Operativa Aplicades i Qualitat es_ES
dc.description.bibliographicCitation Zarzo Castelló, M. (2013). What is a fresh scent in perfumery? Perceptual freshness iscorrelated with substantivity. Sensors. 13(1):463-483. doi:10.3390/s130100463 es_ES
dc.description.accrualMethod S es_ES
dc.relation.publisherversion http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/s130100463 es_ES
dc.description.upvformatpinicio 463 es_ES
dc.description.upvformatpfin 483 es_ES
dc.type.version info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion es_ES
dc.description.volume 13 es_ES
dc.description.issue 1 es_ES
dc.relation.senia 231922
dc.identifier.pmid 23275083 en_EN
dc.identifier.pmcid PMC3574685 en_EN
dc.description.references Zarzo, M., & Stanton, D. T. (2009). Understanding the underlying dimensions in perfumers’ odor perception space as a basis for developing meaningful odor maps. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71(2), 225-247. doi:10.3758/app.71.2.225 es_ES
dc.description.references Rudolfi, T. A., Schedrina, M. M., & Mindlin, L. O. (1988). Determination of the evaporation rate of essential oils and perfumery compositions using gas chromatography. Chromatographia, 25(6), 520-522. doi:10.1007/bf02324825 es_ES
dc.description.references Zarzo, M. (2008). Relevant psychological dimensions in the perceptual space of perfumery odors. Food Quality and Preference, 19(3), 315-322. doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2007.10.007 es_ES
dc.description.references Chastrette, M., Laumer, J.-Y. de S., & Sauvegrain, P. (1991). Analysis of a system of description of odors by means of four different multivariate statistical methods. Chemical Senses, 16(1), 81-93. doi:10.1093/chemse/16.1.81 es_ES
dc.description.references The Rosace of Odors (Firmenich, 1972)http://membres.multimania.fr/sensonet/langa/rosace-firmenich.html es_ES
dc.description.references http://www.perfumerbook.com/ Scent%20Pyramid.pdf es_ES
dc.description.references Abe, H., Kanaya, S., Komukai, T., Takahashi, Y., & Sasaki, S. (1990). Systemization of semantic descriptions of odors. Analytica Chimica Acta, 239, 73-85. doi:10.1016/s0003-2670(00)83837-4 es_ES
dc.description.references Chastrette, M., Elmouaffek, A., & Sauvegrain, P. (1988). A multidimensional statistical study of similarities between 74 notes used in perfumery. Chemical Senses, 13(2), 295-305. doi:10.1093/chemse/13.2.295 es_ES
dc.description.references Wise, P. M. (2000). Quantification of Odor Quality. Chemical Senses, 25(4), 429-443. doi:10.1093/chemse/25.4.429 es_ES
dc.description.references Teixeira, M. A., Rodríguez, O., & Rodrigues, A. E. (2010). Perfumery Radar: A Predictive Tool for Perfume Family Classification. Industrial & Engineering Chemistry Research, 49(22), 11764-11777. doi:10.1021/ie101161v es_ES
dc.description.references http://www-crnl.univ-lyon1.fr/documentation/olfaction/classification_des_parfums/sommaire.htm es_ES
dc.description.references Zarzo, M. (2011). Hedonic Judgments of Chemical Compounds Are Correlated with Molecular Size. Sensors, 11(4), 3667-3686. doi:10.3390/s110403667 es_ES
dc.description.references Yokoyama, K., & Ebisawa, F. (1993). Detection and evaluation of fragrances by human reactions using a chemical sensor based on adsorbate detection. Analytical Chemistry, 65(6), 673-677. doi:10.1021/ac00054a004 es_ES
dc.description.references Stevenson, R. J., & Boakes, R. A. (2003). A mnemonic theory of odor perception. Psychological Review, 110(2), 340-364. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.110.2.340 es_ES
dc.description.references Ayabe-Kanamura, S., Schicker, I., Laska, M., Hudson, R., Distel, H., Kobayakawa, T., & Saito, S. (1998). Differences in Perception of Everyday Odors: a Japanese-German Cross-cultural Study. Chemical Senses, 23(1), 31-38. doi:10.1093/chemse/23.1.31 es_ES
dc.description.references Chrea, C., Valentin, D., Sulmont-Rossé, C., Ly Mai, H., Hoang Nguyen, D., & Abdi, H. (2004). Culture and odor categorization: agreement between cultures depends upon the odors. Food Quality and Preference, 15(7-8), 669-679. doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2003.10.005 es_ES
dc.description.references Kirk-Smith, M. D., & Booth, D. A. (1987). Chemoreception in human behaviour: experimental analysis of the social effects of fragrances. Chemical Senses, 12(1), 159-166. doi:10.1093/chemse/12.1.159 es_ES
dc.description.references Schaal, B., Marlier, L., & Soussignan, R. (1998). Olfactory function in the human fetus: Evidence from selective neonatal responsiveness to the odor of amniotic fluid. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112(6), 1438-1449. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.112.6.1438 es_ES
dc.description.references Mennella, J. A., Jagnow, C. P., & Beauchamp, G. K. (2001). Prenatal and Postnatal Flavor Learning by Human Infants. PEDIATRICS, 107(6), e88-e88. doi:10.1542/peds.107.6.e88 es_ES
dc.description.references Wallace, K. J., & Rosen, J. B. (2000). Predator odor as an unconditioned fear stimulus in rats: Elicitation of freezing by trimethylthiazoline, a component of fox feces. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114(5), 912-922. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.114.5.912 es_ES
dc.description.references Rossiter, K. J. (1996). Structure−Odor Relationships. Chemical Reviews, 96(8), 3201-3240. doi:10.1021/cr950068a es_ES
dc.description.references Ferdenzi, C., Schirmer, A., Roberts, S. C., Delplanque, S., Porcherot, C., Cayeux, I., … Grandjean, D. (2011). Affective dimensions of odor perception: A comparison between Swiss, British, and Singaporean populations. Emotion, 11(5), 1168-1181. doi:10.1037/a0022853 es_ES
dc.description.references Kobayakawa, K., Kobayakawa, R., Matsumoto, H., Oka, Y., Imai, T., Ikawa, M., … Sakano, H. (2007). Innate versus learned odour processing in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nature, 450(7169), 503-508. doi:10.1038/nature06281 es_ES
dc.description.references Chrea, C., Grandjean, D., Delplanque, S., Cayeux, I., Le Calve, B., Aymard, L., … Scherer, K. R. (2008). Mapping the Semantic Space for the Subjective Experience of Emotional Responses to Odors. Chemical Senses, 34(1), 49-62. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjn052 es_ES
dc.description.references PROST, C., GUEN, S. L., COURCOUX, P., & DEMAIMAY, M. (2001). SIMILARITIES AMONG 40 PURE ODORANT COMPOUNDS EVALUATED BY CONSUMERS. Journal of Sensory Studies, 16(6), 551-565. doi:10.1111/j.1745-459x.2001.tb00320.x es_ES
dc.description.references SCHIFFMAN, S., ROBINSON, D. E., & ERICKSON, R. P. (1977). MULTIDIMENSIONAL SCALING OF ODORANTS: EXAMINATION OF PSYCHOLOGICAL AND PHYSICOCHEMICAL DIMENSIONS. Chemical Senses, 2(3), 375-390. doi:10.1093/chemse/2.3.375 es_ES
dc.description.references HARPER, R. (1975). SOME CHEMICALS REPRESENTING PARTICULAR ODOUR QUALITIES. Chemical Senses, 1(3), 353-357. doi:10.1093/chemse/1.3.353 es_ES
dc.description.references Stoddart, D. M. (1986). The Role of Olfaction in the Evolution of Human Sexual Biology: An Hypothesis. Man, 21(3), 514. doi:10.2307/2803099 es_ES
dc.description.references Ou, L.-C., Ronnier Luo, M., Sun, P.-L., Hu, N.-C., Chen, H.-S., Guan, S.-S., … Richter, K. (2010). A cross-cultural comparison of colour emotion for two-colour combinations. Color Research & Application, 37(1), 23-43. doi:10.1002/col.20648 es_ES


Este ítem aparece en la(s) siguiente(s) colección(ones)

Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem